Skip to main content

The Primate Cultural Significance Index: applications with Popoluca Indigenous people at Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve

Abstract

Background

The study of the cultural significance (CS) of biodiversity provides key information to develop conservation strategies consistent with traditions and perceptions of human communities. In Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve (TBR) in Mexico, the mantled howler monkeys (Alouatta palliata mexicana) and the black-handed spider monkeys (Ateles geoffroyi vellerosus) have historically coexisted with Popoluca Indigenous Peoples. This study sought to determine how the presence of a natural protected area (TBR location) and a range of sociodemographic factors (gender, age, origin, language proficiency, education level, religion) relate to the CS held by the Popoluca Indigenous People in relation to these two endangered primate species.

Methods

The first Primate Cultural Significance Index (PCSI) was designed as a composed index of 11 cultural variables (sub-indices) and was applied randomly to a representative size sample of people over 15 years old in two Popolucas communities, one within the TBR (Piedra Labrada = 81 people) and another outside (Los Mangos = 91). U Mann–Whitney tests were used to compare the PCSI between communities and Generalized Linear Models (GLM) to evaluate the sociodemographic factors of participants that influenced the sub-indices in the PCSI.

Results

The cultural significance of spider monkeys held by the Popolucas was higher for the community within the TBR than for the community outside, while for howler monkeys it was higher outside. For both primate species across the two communities, the most relevant sub-indices were (1) interest in conservation and (2) touristic significance of primates. Sociodemographic factors of participants influenced nine sub-indices of cultural significance out of the possible 10 sub-indices applied for each primate species. The demographic factors that most influenced each sub-index for both species were location and gender.

Conclusions

The main differences found between communities may be linked to the conservation and sustainable development programs promoted by the reserve, as well as the greater persistence of Popolucan ancestral traditions within the boundaries of the reserve. We recommend that conservation efforts should focus on people less interested about primate conservation (women, non-natives and residents outside the reserve), and turn to the leadership of people more interested (native men who reside inside the reserve).

Background

Currently, it is difficult to find wild primate populations without some human influence [1]. For this reason, it is critical to study interactions between nonhuman primates (hereafter primates) and people [2]. Those interactions and their effect on animal conservation are the main object of study in ethnoprimatology [3]. Ethnobiological studies show that primates play a significant cultural role in many societies around the world [4, 5]. In Southeast Asia, primates are sold for the pet trade or to be consumed as medicine. They are also given important symbolism; are seen as holders of moral principles and their protection is associated with the sacred sites they habit [6,7,8]. Throughout Mesoamerica and South America, primates have also been used for a range purposes, including as food, medicine, and as raw materials to make cultural artifacts. In Mesoamerica, primates are sold by local people into the wildlife trade in order to resolve economic needs [9,10,11,12,13,14,15]. Likewise, primates are important in the symbolism and cosmovision of several ancestral cultures of the Neotropics [10, 16,17,18,19].

The value and role that any species plays in human cultures is defined as Cultural Significance – CS [20]. Culturally significant species are defined as those used as food or medicine, playing a role in religious practices or group mythology, or that have negative connotations (e.g., poisonous, invasive) [21]. To quantify the role of any organism in a particular culture, Turner (1988) proposed the Cultural Significance Index (CSI). He claimed that, despite the limitations that this index could have, it is less subjective and allows researchers to quantify cultural significance in a systematic and impartial way [21]. Nevertheless, the success of using this kind of index depends on the clarity and accuracy of its measurement, and therefore it is critical to utilize solid indicators or variables as well as to have a deep knowledge of the study context [20, 22].

Since the development and use of Turner’s first CSI, the cultural significance of organisms has been measured using indices in different ways. One of the simplest, common and most accurate of these is the Frequency of Mention Index [23, 24], which has been applied successfully to plants and fungi. CS has also been measured by using composed indices, considering value of use, cultural consensus, and data aggregation, among others [22, 25, 26]. Currently, there are no CSI designed and applied for primates only, but some are in use for vertebrates in general [27]. Studies to date have shown that the presence of natural protected areas and various social and demographic factors, such as gender, age, education level and religion, can influence perceptions and cultural values about biodiversity [28,29,30].

On the American continent, Los Tuxtlas region in southern Mexico represents the boreal limit of the tropical forest [31] and the most northern habitat for two of the three primate species present in Mexico, the mantled howler monkey (Alouatta palliata mexicana) and the spider monkey (Ateles geoffroyi vellerosus) [32, 33]. These primates have historically coexisted with the Popoluca Indigenous Peoples, the native ethnic group of Los Tuxtlas region [34, 35]. Traditionally, the Popolucas have used the natural resources guided by their traditions, satisfying their livelihood needs without negatively affecting the ecosystems [36]. However, these traditions are at risk of disappearing due to biodiversity loss, and recent socio-economic and demographic conditions [36, 37].

The original forest cover in Los Tuxtlas region, which was once 88%, has been lost principally as a result of cattle farming and agriculture [38]. At present, 60% of the landcover is for human use and the rest consists of fragmented forest [39]. Since 1937, several different conservation programs have been established in the region, but it was not until 1998 that 155.122 ha of the area was declared as Biosphere Reserve [36]. The aim of Biosphere Reserves of UNESCO is to harmonize interactions between people and nature for conservation and sustainable development, taking concerns and community participation into account in the management of protected areas [40]. In Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve, conservation policies have thus sought to balance biodiversity conservation with local development, creating opportunities for human communities [41]. Around the world it is increasingly recognized that to achieve greater effectiveness in management and conservation of protected natural areas, local perception and cultural values must be considered [42]. With regard to primates, the study of the cultural significance provides key information to develop conservation strategies consistent with traditions and perceptions of people who share territories with these species [43].

This study aimed to: (1) measure and compare the cultural significance (CS) for two endangered primate species (A. geoffroyi and A. palliata) between two Indigenous Popoluca communities (one within Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve (TBR), and another out of it); (2) evaluate the possible importance of social and demographic factors (gender, age, origin, perceived language proficiency, education level, occupation, and location) on the cultural significance index (CSI) of primates in the two Popolucan communities. In general, Biosphere Reserves seek to incorporate the local communities concerns and participation into management. For Popolucas, the TBR represents an opportunity for their traditional resource management to be integrated. Thus, we hypothesized that the CSI primates will play a more significant cultural role within the Popoluca community within the TBR than outside.

Methods

Study area and human communities

This study was conducted in 2017 in two Popoluca Indigenous communities in the state of Veracruz, Mexico: Piedra Labrada (inside the TBR) and Los Mangos (outside the TBR), (Fig. 1). The TBR is at the limit of the northern distribution of the tropical rainforest on the American continent [31] with an altitude ranging from 0 to 1780 masl and covering an area of 155,122 ha [44]. The climate is warm and humid (mean annual temperature = 25 °C, mean annual precipitation = 4 900 mm) [45] and the vegetation type is evergreen rainforest [46].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Study area and communities inside and outside of Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve (TBR), Veracruz, Mexico. Piedra Labrada = Community inside the TBR; Los Mangos = Community outside the TBR

The community of Piedra Labrada (18°23′40" N, 94°46′35" W) is located inside the TBR, in the management zone designated for “Sustainable Use of Ecosystems”, which is a part of the buffer zone. Piedra Labrada has a population of 510 inhabitants of which 56% are self-identified Indigenous [47]. The community of Los Mangos (18°13′32" N, 95°08′22" W), located 16 km outside the reserve, has a population of 4131 inhabitants of which 16% are self-identified as Indigenous [47].

The native ethnic group of Los Tuxtlas is the Popoluca. They consider themselves as the children of Homshuk, the god of corn [34]. The Popolucas’ native language, Popolucan, is spoken in their communities and many pre-hispanic traditions are maintained, some of which are related to the management of natural resources [35]. However, due to the economic changes and growing demographic pressures from outside populations, as well as the influence of the Catholic and Protestant churches, these traditions are currently at risk [35, 36].

Sequential study design

In this study, we adhered to the code of ethics by the Latin American Society of Ethnobiology [48]. The research consisted in four sequential phases: (1) Preliminary visit: the project was presented and the permission to conduct the research was requested of local leaders and authorities. (2) Period of qualitative information: semi-structured interviews were conducted with environmental and traditional leaders, elders and people living close to monkeys, obtaining information on their perceptions and cultural aspects regarding human-primate interactions such as the role of primates in the myths and legends, rituals, popular beliefs and archaeological representations, as well as their use at a medicinal, nutritional and economic level, and as pets. Those qualitative aspects were published in Pinto-Marroquin and Serio-Silva (2020) [49]. (3) Questionnaire design: based on the information obtained during the previous phase, variables reflecting the cultural importance of primates were identified and the Primate Cultural Significance Index (PCSI) and its questionnaire were designed to quantitatively assess the cultural significance (Additional file 1). The Edible Mushrooms Cultural Significance Index (EMCSI) [22, 50] and the Cultural Food Significance Index (CFSI) [51] were adapted for this study. A pilot survey was conducted with 14 men and 14 women from the Popoluca population. (4) Data collection: the permission to survey and the consent to publish responses from each person were granted before conducting the study. The questionnaire consisted of 22 closed multiple choice questions regarding cultural variables (sub-indices). The first question inquired about which primates were present in the locality, and photographs of A. geoffroyi and A. palliata were shown to verify their identity. Regardless if one of the species was mentioned or not, all of the same questions were asked for both species. One last question measured the importance of Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve and was not included when calculating PCSI. The sample size (how many people were surveyed) for each community was calculated on the survey system web page (http://www.surveysystem.com/sscalc.htm#one) using a confidence level of 95% and a confidence interval of 10. The sample size for the total population over 15 years old inside the TBR (Piedra Labrada = 356 inhabitants) was 76 and 93 outside (Los Mangos = 2722 inhabitants) (see social and demographic factors of the participants in Table 2).

Primate Cultural Significance Index (PCSI)

The Primate Cultural Significance Index (PCSI) was composed of 11 sub-indices (cultural variables) influencing CS:\({\text{PCSI}} = {\text{ OMF }}\left( {{\text{MED }} + {\text{ PET }} + {\text{ COM }} + {\text{ FOOD }} + {\text{ TOUR }} + {\text{ CSV}} + {\text{ EMO }} + {\text{ CONS }} + {\text{ ECO }} + {\text{ ABU}}} \right)\), where OMF = Order of mention frequency, MED = Medicinal significance, PET = Significance as companion animal, COM = Commercial significance, FOOD = Food significance, TOUR = Touristic significance, CSV = Significance in cosmovision, EMO = Emotional significance, CONS = Interest in conservation, ECO = Ecological significance, ABU = Perceived abundance.

In the PCSI, the values of each sub-index (except OMF) corresponds to the score average of the answers obtained from all the participants in each community for each species. The general index can reach a maximum value of 1000 (see scores in Table 1). The values for each sub-index ranged from zero (0) to ten (10). Some sub-indices were also composed of indicators, which were added, resulting in the total value of the sub-index. For these indicators, the maximum value of each sub-index was divided among the number of indicators that compose it, giving each indicator an equitable value. In turn, the possible answers of each indicator were scored giving higher values to the alternatives that represented greater importance (ordinal variable) (Table 1). The Order of Mention Frequency (OMF) [52] was modified from the Order of Mention (OM) proposed by Pieroni (2001) and Garibay-Orijel et al. (2007) [22, 51]. As this study had only two species, the OMF refers to the sum of the times (relative frequency) that each primate species was first mentioned spontaneously by the participants when asked about the primate species present in each community, divided by the total number of participants:

$${\text{OMF}} = \left( {{\text{No}}.\;{\text{of}}\;{\text{first}}\;{\text{mentions}}\;{\text{for}}\;{\text{each }}\;{\text{species/No}}.\;{\text{of}}\;{\text{paticipants}}} \right){ }10$$
Table 1 Values of sub-indices, indicators and possible answers in the PCSI

In addition, the order of mention frequency value goes from 0 to 1, therefore it must be multiplied by 10 to adjust its values to the same scale as the other sub-indices [22].

In the PCSI, the OMF is multiplied by the other sub-indices to amplify the differences between the species [22] as several authors agree that the frequency and the order of mention are accurate variables to measure the cultural significance of species [23, 50]. This reduces biases and subjectivity [24]. Given that the PCSI is a composed additive index, the potential effect of each sub-index can be masked. We will discuss the performance of the sub-indices independently since compound indices should be a tool to separate, analyze and understand cultural significance phenomena, and also a technique to estimate it [22].

Perceived significance of Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve

The perceived significance of Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve in the daily life of the inhabitants of each community was measured independently to the PCSI, on a 0–10 scale. The values for the answers were: Very important = 10, Important = 6.67, Less important = 3.33, Not important = 0.

Data analysis

We compared the sub-indices of the PCSI between the communities and primate species using the U Mann–Whitney test. We evaluated the social and demographic factors of the participants (predictor variables: see social and demographic factors in Table 2) that influenced each sub-index (response variables were the sub-indices of cultural significance, Table 1: Medicinal, Companion animal, Commercial, Food, Touristic, Cosmovision, Emotional, Interest in conservation, Ecological, Perceived abundance) for spider and howler monkeys using Generalized Linear Models (GLMs) fit with gamma error distributions and inverse link (20 models). The OMF sub-index was not included in this portion of the analysis. To select the combination of predictor variables best fitting each dataset, we used the Akaike’s information criterion [53]. The model with the lowest ΔAIC score was considered the best-fitting model. However, in cases where the next best-fitting model was within two AIC points, indicating a plausible alternative, we selected the most parsimonious model (i.e., the model with the fewest predictors) [53]. With this information we calculated the Akaike weight (wi) for each model, which ranged from 0 to 1 and was interpreted as the relative probability that a model is the best among all those evaluated. This means that models with higher wi values will be the most probable and will have a greater explanatory power for the response factors. For the selected models, we calculated the relation between variables extracting the P value and slope from the GLM test. The GLMs were performed with the nlme package [54] and for fitting maximum likelihood models we used bbmle package [55] within the statistical program R (version 3.2.0) [56].

Table 2 Social and demographic factors and percentage of participants within and outside the TBR

Results

Ethnographic information

Primates do not appear in the myths or legends of Popolucas. However, there are a number of popular beliefs associated with both howler and spider monkeys. Howler monkeys are believed to kidnap and sexually abuse women and spider monkeys are related to supernatural beings. Nevertheless, both species are associated with joy and fun. The howler monkeys are widely considered to provide a meteorological service by predicting the arrival of rainy or dry seasons when howling. Petroglyphs created by the ancient Olmecs with figures of monkeys were found in the Piedra Labrada community. The Popolucas do not traditionally consume primates as food, but they did use spider monkeys medicinally and, in the past, used to keep the young of this species as companion animals to later release them. On an economic level, spider monkeys have been extracted for consumption or to be used as bait for fishing by non-indigenous people in the area. Popolucas in the communities of study consider that the populations of both species have decreased due to deforestation and hunting. Additionally, they perceive the loss of natural vegetation due to cattle ranching, commercial agriculture and timber extraction as the main threats to primates. They view the sale of traditional indigenous lands to non-indigenous settlers as an indirect threat. Finally, the Popolucas of these communities express a willingness to conserve primates so that young people and future generations may know them. They consider that tourism is a worthwhile strategy to aid non-human primate conservation, but to offer tourism services, they will need external support. For detailed ethnographic information about cultural interactions of Popolucas with primates see Pinto-Marroquin & Serio-Silva (2020) [49].

Primate Cultural Significance Index (PCSI)

The results of the PCSI (Table 3) showed that spider monkeys play a more important role in the Popoluca community inside the TBR (S-PCSI = 151.9) rather than outside the TBR (S-PCSI = 60.9), which supports our hypothesis. On the other hand, the lower PCSI value for howler monkeys inside (H-PCSI = 209.5) rather than outside the reserve (H-PCSI = 266.1) does not support our hypothesis. For both species in both communities, the sub-indices with higher values were interest in conservation (CONS) and touristic significance (TOUR). The significantly higher sub-indices values in the community inside rather than outside the TBR, for both species, were interest in conservation (CONS), touristic significance (TOUR) and perceived abundance (ABU). Inside the TBR, for spider monkeys, the higher values were the significance as companion animal (PET) and emotional significance (EMO). For the same species, but in the community outside the TBR, the higher values were for medicinal significance (MED) and food significance (FOOD).

Table 3 Results and comparisons of the PCSI for both primate species between communities

In general, the sub-indices with the lowest value were commercial significance (COM) and ecological significance (ECO). Howler monkeys showed a greater value than spider monkeys in the overall result of the PCSI. The sub-indices comparison between species in both communities indicated that howler monkeys had higher significance in the cosmovision (CSV) and perceived abundance (ABU), and lower medicinal (MED) and food (FOOD) significance; whereas spider monkeys showed lower significance in cosmovision (CSV).

Perceived significance of Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve (TBR-P)

Significant differences were found in the perceived significance of TBR between the community inside (Piedra Labrada, TBR-P = 6.91) and outside the reserve (Los Mangos, TBR-P = 5.11) (u = 2556, p = 0.0004, n = 171). Most participants inside the TBR considered the reserve as important in people’s daily life (80%). In the community outside the TBR, half of the participants recognized the reserve as important, while the remaining considered it as not important (28%) or ignored the existence of the reserve (22%). In both communities the participants acknowledged the reserve function for conservation of wild animals and forest, water supply, climate, and air quality regulation; as well as its role in environmental education.

Social and demographic factors influencing cultural significance of primates

The results of GLMs (Table 4) showed that social and demographic factors influenced nine sub-indices of cultural importance out of 10 tested per species; likewise, 15 models were significant out of 20 tested (six for spider monkeys; nine for howler monkeys). In spider monkeys the sub-indices (see names, abbreviations and calculations in Table 1) that were explained by social and demographic factors were: FOOD, TOUR, EMO, ICON, ECO, ABU; while for howler monkeys were: MED, PET, FOOD, TOUR, CSV, EMO, ICON, ECO AND ABU.

Table 4 Results of GLMs for social and demographic factors that influenced the sub-indices of the PCSI

The social and demographic factors that most explained each sub-index of cultural significance for both primate species were the location of communities with respect to the TBR (TBR location; nine sub-indices out of 20 proved), gender (nine sub-indices out of 20 proved). The TBR location factor in spider monkeys influenced the sub-indices FOOD, TOUR, EMO, CONS and ABU; while for howler monkeys influenced TOUR, CSV, CONS and ABU. The gender factor in spider monkeys influenced FOOD, CONS, ECO and ABU; while in howler monkeys influenced MED, FOOD, EMO, ECO and ABU. Detailed results of social and demographic factors influencing the sub-indices are presented in Table 4.

The results of both tests (U Mann–Whitney and GLMs) regarding the influence of Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve on all the sub-indices were consistent with the exception of significance in cosmovision (CSV) for howler monkeys, which was only found in the GLMs.

Discussion

The Primate Cultural Significance Index (PCSI) showed differential cultural relevance for both primate species between Popolucan communities within (Piedra Labrada) and outside (Los Mangos) the Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve (TBR). We found that spider monkeys (A. geoffroyi) were culturally significant inside, while howler monkeys (A. palliata) were viewed as culturally significant outside of the reserve, which partially supports our hypothesis.

The PCSI was strongly influenced by the order of mention frequency (OMF). It has been argued that most mentioned species are often the more abundant or more likely to be seen [24]. When comparing the OMF between communities, the spider monkeys were mentioned more frequently inside the reserve. This may be related to a major abundance due to the fact that their habitat is more connected and conserved within the reserve [39, 57] (Fig. 2), also explaining the higher perceived abundance (ABU) and emotional significance (EMO) for this species. Conversely, the howler monkeys had higher OMF and EMO values outside the reserve, even when their perceived abundance (ABU) was higher inside the reserve. This may be explained by howler monkeys being the only primate recognized as present outside the reserve in the majority of surveys.

“Spider monkeys no longer exist around here. The last time they were seen was around the 70's. The howler monkeys are the only ones present here in Los Mangos”. F. Pascual, Los Mangos, July 2016.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Type of surrounding habitat of the study communities. A inside the TBR (Piedra Labrada). B outside the TBR (Los Mangos). (Photograph A by Jorge Ramos, photograph B by the first author)

Spider monkeys are rarely seen outside the reserve as a result of forest loss and fragmentation that have limited their food resources making this species more vulnerable [57]. Primate demographic studies in Los Tuxtlas reported that howler monkeys are able to survive in more disturbed habitats with small forest fragments including anthropogenic habitats [58]. In contrast, spider monkeys require forests with continuous coverage and more tree diversity [59, 60].

The sub-indices with greater cultural significance inside the reserve for both species were perceived abundance (ABU), interest in conservation (CONS) and touristic significance (TOUR). Primate abundance within Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve (TBR) has remained relatively stable since the 1980s [57, 60] and the forest cover increased by 2402.97 ha (1.55% of TBR area) between 2006 and 2016 [39]. This is probably due to the protected area regulations which ban logging and hunting [61]. The interest in conservation (CONS) and the touristic significance of primates (TOUR), in addition to being more important inside the TBR, were the highest sub-indices for both communities and primate species. It has been estimated that in Los Tuxtlas region, since the Spanish colonization, the population of primates has decreased by 80% with some groups persisting in fragmented and isolated vegetation patches [59, 60]. Spider monkey populations have decreased faster than howler monkeys [60]. This situation, along with the influence of the reserve and of primate conservation education strategies in the region, such as the annual festival Changos y monos tesoros de los Tuxtlas which has been held since 2014, may have motivated people to conserve primates, particularly in communities within the reserve.

“If someone is hunting primates here, we go and report it to the municipal agent. We want these animals to continue to exist so that the children will get to know them”. A. Albino, Piedra Labrada, October 2016.

In the neighboring locality of Catemaco, the economic income generated by tour boats to observe exotic primates (Macaca arctoides) and spider monkeys in the Catemaco lake is more important than other economic activities such as agriculture, livestock farming and working in protected natural areas [62]. This case was mentioned by the participants during this study as an example of how the presence of primates represents an income opportunity.

“In Catemaco many tourists go to see the monkeys, which leaves economic profit to the inhabitants. If tourism here was organized in such a way that visitors would come to see the monkeys by the river, it would be a good economic opportunity for us”. J. González, Los Mangos, July 2016.

Since the 1980s, tourism has been increasingly explored as a way to conserve primates around the globe, which in consequence protects their habitat and generates an income for local people [63]. For example, in the Central African Republic, ecotourism has been established with the main attraction of observing groups of gorillas in the wild, economically benefiting the local population and the primate populations themselves [64]. In some temples in Southeast Asia, monkeys are an important attraction for visitors; local human communities receive economic benefits from tourism and primates are perceived as culturally important, receiving protection by being associated with sacred sites [7, 65]. Tourism has also been implemented as a strategy in the Colombian Amazon in Tikuna territories where conservation centers have been established. There, tourists can see rescued monkeys and in doing so, contribute to the local economy and benefits people [12].

For spider monkeys, we found higher cultural significance within the reserve regarding emotional significance (EMO) and significance as companion animal (PET). This result may be related to positive emotions associated with a higher possibility of sighting them (greater perceived abundance ABU), a higher desire that this species continues to exist (CONS), and Popolucas’ ancestral tradition that encourages allowing spider monkeys to roam free around their houses.

“A long time ago in my family, we had three young spider monkeys. Once they were tamed, we left them loose around the house and when they were older, they returned to the forest” A. Albino, Piedra Labrada, October 2016.

Since the language proficiency level is an indicator of cultural strength [66] and there is higher perceived language proficiency in the community within the TBR (Table 2), Popolucan ancestral traditions, such as this one, even when they are banned, will continue. In 2003 in Mexico, most primates kept as pets (70%) corresponded to spider monkeys [13] and in Mesoamerica they have been commonly kept as pets because they are more tamable and versatile, while howlers are difficult to tame [16, 19]. The appropriation of monkeys as companion animals is a common practice in Latin America, an example of this is documented for the Maijuna of the Ecuadorian Amazon [10], among Yanomami groups from Brazil and Venezuela [9] and among Lokono, Kari'na and Warao groups in Guyana [14] where, commonly, people “adopt” infants after the mothers are hunted.

Conversely, the higher sub-indices for spider monkeys outside the TBR were related to their extraction (food significance FOOD and medicinal significance MED) and may be explained by the fact that law enforcement in the reserve prevents wildlife hunting [36]. However, traditionally, Popolucas do not eat spider monkeys despite the fact that they describe the meat as edible (Table 3) while, in other traditional societies, the genus Ateles spp. is the most appreciated primate for human consumption [19].

"None of us Popolucas eat monkeys. We were told that it is not good to eat them because they are people... We have heard that spider monkey meat tastes good". J. Albino, Piedra Labrada, October 2016.

Moreover, there is a mestizo tradition of consuming spider monkeys in the nearby municipality of Catemaco, where their meat is highly valued, influencing their use as food in Los Mangos. Some cases have been documented in which people do not consume primate meat (for example in certain areas of the Brazilian Amazon and on the border between Colombia and Venezuela) because they perceive these organisms as similar to human children [19, 67, 68], however the consumption of meat from different species of monkeys is common among indigenous cultures throughout the Neotropics. The consumption of various primate species has been reported among the Lacandones of Chiapas, Mexico [24]; among the Maijuna of the Peruvian Amazon [10]; among the Waoriani of the Ecuadorian Amazon [69]; among the Tikuna in Colombia [70], and among the Tacana of Bolivia [15], just to mention a few examples. Meat is regularly boiled, roasted or smoked for consumption and is generally perceived as excellent tasting meat, even preferred over other wild game [10, 14, 15]. Among the Tikuna of southeastern Colombia, the wooly monkey is consumed as a ritual meal [12]. On other occasions, the meat is not destined for household-consumption but is brought to local markets to be sold as bushmeat, where such sale can generate cash to satisfy other household needs.

We also found a higher medicinal significance (MED) outside the reserve for the spider monkey probably because this species is considered by study participants as the only cure for many diseases. The use of monkeys in traditional medicine is also a widespread practice throughout the world, where the use of more than 100 species of primates in ethnomedical practices or magical-religious rituals has been recorded [11, 71]. In Latin America, for example, the use of howler throat sacs has been recorded to cure laryngitis among the Tikunas [12] and the use of howler hairs to counter scorpion stinging and to ward off evil spirits among different groups in Guyana has also been documented [14].

For howler monkeys we found that their significance in cosmovision (CSV) was influenced by the participants outside the reserve, where the Popolucan cultural influence is not as strong. Also, by people belonging to protestant churches, suggesting that reported stories and beliefs are not ancestrally popolucans. Since the 1960’s, the number of Protestants churches in Mexico has grown exponentially [72], fostering new traditions [73] and focusing particularly on reported stories by women [74]. In this study, many women narrated that howler monkeys chase and try to kidnap them (30% of participants). Legends relating to women and primates extend beyond howler monkey species in other cultures. Among the Takana of the Bolivian Amazon, it is commonly said that spider monkeys can kidnap women from villages and take them to the jungle [15]. Participants widely conveyed that howler monkeys can predict and announce weather variations when they vocalize (80%). This ethnobiological knowledge has been described throughout the geographic range of this genera, for example in Argentina by the Qom (Toba) People of the Gran Chaco and in Guyana by the Lokono, Kari’na, and Warao Peoples [14, 75].

“When the weather's going to change, the monkeys start screaming. They announce when it is going to rain, when the sun is going to warm, when it is going to hit a sur (dry season) or a norte (rainy and cold season)”. V. Lázaro, Los Mangos, July 2016.

The demographic factor that most influenced the cultural significance (sub-indices, Table 4) of primates after the TBR location was gender. Males in our study especially influenced the perceived abundance and emotional significance for both species, and the interest in conservation for spider monkeys. Men also had influence in the food significance of spider monkeys in the community outside the reserve. This may be explained by the fact that men do crop work near the forest, increasing their interactions with primates, while women are mainly in charge of domestic tasks in their houses, sometimes helping in the fields. Traditionally, Popolucan adult men are the ones who access the forest to hunt and fish [76]. We also found a higher ecological knowledge (ECO) among men compared to that of women. This pattern has been observed in other countries in the region such as in Costa Rica where it was reported that although women visit the forest as often as men, they may show less knowledge about ecology and conservation [77], which, in turn, can affect their attitudes and values towards the environment [78]. In southern Mexico, a study has shown that when educational programs promoting primate conservation are offered equally between boys and girls, gender differences in ecological knowledge about monkeys disappear [79]. Native participants influenced higher interest in conservation for howler monkeys and emotional significance for both species, which reflects a sense of ownership, and a greater concern and appreciation of their local biodiversity than immigrants from other areas.

The differences we found between communities can be attributed to the conservation and education programs implemented by the reserve managers. These differences, however may also be due to uneven socioeconomic development which is, in part a consequence of government and NGO investment in and promotion of sustainable economic development inside the boundaries of the reserve [36]. In addition, we found a higher perception of the importance of the protected area (TBR) by participants within the reserve rather than outside the reserve, as expected. Studies in Asia and Africa also have reported that people living closer to natural parks perceived more benefits and had a positive attitude towards conservation and the protected areas [42, 80]. Likewise, at various locations across Latin America, conservation actions undertaken within protected areas has led to a decrease in hunting and a reduction in the consumption of primates for food or traditional remedies [12, 70, 81] thereby contributing to the recovery of primate populations.

Conclusion

Spider monkeys play a more culturally significant role in the Popoluca community inside the reserve (Piedra Labrada) when compared to the community outside (Los Mangos). Howler monkeys are more culturally important outside the TBR likely because they are the only species recognized as currently present in this community. The sub-indices with greater CS inside the reserve for both species were mainly related to conservation, while the indices that were more significant outside of the reserve were related to extraction and consumption of spider monkeys. The social groups that perceived a higher CS of primates were men and interview participants living inside the reserve. The main differences in CS of primates between communities may be tied to the conservation and sustainable development programs carried out inside the reserve and which likely influence attitudes towards primates inside the reserve. The Popolucan ancestral traditions are more preserved within the community inside the reserve, and thus also likely has an important effect on the differing CS of primates between the two study communities.

The high interest in conservation and ecotourism in both communities represents a strategic opportunity for primate conservation in Los Tuxtlas region. Therefore, conservation efforts should encourage people to remain on their ancestral lands, by supporting sustainable management and economic opportunities for communities. Such programs could follow the pattern of other successful conservation programs which are considered to be long term, small-scale, and relatively low-budget [82]. To implement responsible tourism programs, it is essential to consider that tourists can expose primates to physiological stress and anxiety, producing changes in their behavioral patterns and also exposing humans and primates alike to injury and disease [83]. Tourism activities should be implemented promoting conservation education, avoiding direct interactions, and minimizing impacts on the ecosystems. Traditional values of indigenous people must also be considered to avoid conflicts of interest, project failures, and negative effects on their culture [84]. In order to ensure long-term sustainability for ecotourism, community participation and the generation of profits are essential [85]. Primate conservation, education and ecotourism programs in Los Tuxtlas should be gender inclusive and focus on strengthening ecological knowledge and cultural traditions in the social groups less interested about conservation such as people outside the reserve, women, and non-natives. These programs should also focus on taking advantage of the ecological knowledge and interest in conservation of native men inside the reserve for leadership.

The conservation of non-human primates is an objective that requires theoretical and methodological frameworks that go beyond the dictates of disciplines such as Conservation Biology [86]. From the humanities, epistemic concepts such as “post humanism” have been proposed as an attempt to include non-human beings in the social sciences and humanities [87, 88], or the so-called “multispecies ethnographies”, that pose a blurring of the ontological line between human and nature [89]. For Robinson and Remis (2018) [86], multispecies ethnography can foster a more holistic and transdisciplinary research, and a framework that allows highlighting the wide and intricate networks in which primates, humans and non-humans, as well as other organisms coexist and interact. Other perspectives such as Evolutionary Ethnobiology theorize about how the changes generated by humans to the environment influence the evolutionary process of other organisms, giving feedback in a dialectical way to the evolution of humans [90].

Ethnoprimatologists have been exploring holistic paths, for example promoting a common field of intersection between anthropology and primatology for biological and cultural conservation [7, 91]; and also recognizing the blurring boundaries between primates and humans through the deep understanding of culturally complex relationships among indigenous peoples [17, 92]. There are also multiple examples of current transdisciplinary research in ethnoprimatology that go from collaborative research among anthropologists, biologists, and indigenous peoples [9], the interpretation of ancestral codes, values, and links in the collective unconscious of people for conservation [93] to the understanding of primates as “other-than human persons” from a cognitive point of view [94] or from a multispecies perspective [89]. Finally, as we find in this study, quantitative ethnoprimatology, particularly cultural significance indexes, provides an analytical framework for synthesizing information and patterns across socio-ecological systems. The cultural significance index highlights the elements that people consider most important in their relationship with primates and thereby can facilitate the design of culturally and socially relevant conservation strategies, strategies that are more likely to be effective once implemented.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

ABU:

Perceived abundance

AIC:

Akaike information criterion

CFSI:

Cultural Food Significance Index

COM:

Commercial significance

CONANP:

Comisión Nacional de Áreas Naturales Protegidas

CONS:

Interest in conservation

CS:

Cultural significance

CSI:

Cultural Significance Index

CSV:

Significance in cosmovision

ECO:

Ecological significance

EMCSI:

Edible Mushrooms Cultural Significance Index

EMO:

Emotional significance

FOOD:

Food significance

GLM:

Generalized linear model

IMSS:

Instituto Mexicano del Seguro Social

INPI:

Instituto Nacional de los Pueblos Indígenas

MED:

Medicinal significance

OMF:

Order of mention frequency

PET:

Significance as companion animal

PCSI:

Primate Cultural Significance Index

TBR:

Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve

TOUR:

Touristic significance

UNESCO:

United Nations Educational, Scientific and Cultural Organization

References

  1. Riley EP. Contemporary primatology in anthropology: beyond the epistemological abyss. Am Anthropol. 2013;115(3):411–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. McKinney T, Dore KM. The state of ethnoprimatology: its use and potential in today’s primate research. Int J Primatol. 2018;39(5):730–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Sponsel LE. The human niche in Amazonia: explorations in ethnoprimatology. In: Kinzey WG, editor. New world primates: ecology, evolution, and behavior. New York: Aldine de Gruyter; 1997. p. 143–65.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Waller MT. Ethnoprimatology [Internet]. Waller MT, editor. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2016. 422 p. (Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/978-3-319-30469-4.

  5. Urbani B, Lizarralde M. Neotropical ethnoprimatology: indigenous peoples’ perceptions of and interactions with nonhuman primates. Urbani B, Lizarralde M, editors. Cham, Switzerland: Springer; 2020.

  6. Lee PC, Priston NEC. Human attitudes to primates: perceptions of pests, conflict and consequences for primate conservation. Commensalism Confl human-primate interface. 2005;4:1–23.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Loudon JE, Howells ME, Fuentes A. The importance of integrative anthropology: a preliminary investigation employing primatological and cultural anthropological data collectionmethods in assessing human-monkey co-existence in Bali, Indonesia. Ecol Environ Anthropol. 2006;2(1):1–13.

    Google Scholar 

  8. Nekaris KAI, Shepherd CR, Starr CR, Nijman V. Exploring cultural drivers for wildlife trade via an ethnoprimatological approach: a case study of slender and slow lorises (Loris and Nycticebus) in South and Southeast Asia. Am J Primatol. 2010;72(10):877–86.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Boubli JP, Urbani B, Caballero-Arias H, Shepard GH Jr, Lizarralde M. Primates in the Lives of the Yanomami People of Brazil and Venezuela. In: Urbani B, Lizarralde M, editors. Neotropical ethnoprimatology. Springer: Cham; 2020. p. 199–224.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  10. Roncal CM, Bowler M, Gilmore MP. The ethnoprimatology of the Maijuna of the Peruvian Amazon and implications for primate conservation. J Ethnobiol Ethnomed. 2018;14(1):1–16.

    Google Scholar 

  11. Sampaio MB, Souto A, Schiel N. Ethnoprimatology. In: Albuquerque UP, Nóbrega Alves RR, editors. Introduction to ethnobiology. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2016. p. 63–8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/978-3-319-28155-1_10.

  12. Maldonado AM, Waters S. Ethnoprimatology of the Tikuna in the southern Colombian amazon. In: Urbani B, Lizarralde M, editors. Neotropical ethnoprimatology. Cham: Springer; 2020. p. 89–107.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  13. Duarte-Quiroga A, Estrada A. Primates as pets in Mexico City: an assessment of the species involved, source of origin, and general aspects of treatment. Am J Primatol. 2003;61(2):53–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Rybka K. Linguistic, cultural, and environmental aspects of ethnoprimatological knowledge among the Lokono, Kari’na, and Warao of the Moruca River (Guyana). In: Urbani B, Lizarralde M, editors. Neotropical ethnoprimatology. Cham: Springer; 2020. p. 239–62.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  15. Townsend WR, Wallace RB, Lara-Delgado K, Miranda-Chumacero G. Importance of primates to tacana indigenous subsistence hunting in the Bolivian amazon. In: Urbani B, Lizarralde M, editors. Neotropical ethnoprimatology. Springer: Cham; 2020. p. 343–62.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  16. Bruner E, Cucina A. Alouatta, Ateles, and the ancient Mesoamerican cultures. J Anthropol Sci. 2005;83:111–7.

    Google Scholar 

  17. Cormier L. A preliminary review of neotropical primates in the subsistence and symbolism of indigenous lowland South American Peoples. Ecol Environ Anthropol. 2006;2(1):14–32.

    Google Scholar 

  18. Urbani B, Cormier LA. The ethnoprimatology of the Howler Monkeys (Alouatta spp .): From Past to Present. 2015;259–80.

  19. Cormier LA, Urbani B. The ethnoprimatology of spider monkeys (Ateles spp.): from past to present. In: Campbell C, editor. Spider monkeys: Behavior, ecology and evolution of the genus Ateles. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2008. p. 377–403.

  20. Hunn E. The utilitarian factor in folk biological classification. Am Anthropol. 1982;84(4):830–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Turner NJ. “The Importance Of A Rose”: evaluating the cultural significance of plants in Thompson and Lillooet interior Salish. Am Anthropol. 1988;90(2):272–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Garibay-Orijel R, Caballero J, Estrada-Torres A, Cifuentes J. Understanding cultural significance, the edible mushrooms case. J Ethnobiol Ethnomed. 2007;3:1–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Heinrich M, Ankli A, Frei B, Weimann C, Sticher O. Medicinal plants in Mexico: healers’ consensus and cultural importance. Soc Sci Med. 1998;47(11):1859–71.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. García del Valle Y, Naranjo EJ, Caballero J, Martorell C, Ruan-Soto F, Enríquez PL. Cultural significance of wild mammals in mayan and mestizo communities of the Lacandon Rainforest, Chiapas, Mexico. J Ethnobiol Ethnomed. 2015;11(1).

  25. Sujarwo W, Caneva G. Using quantitative indices to evaluate the cultural importance of food and nutraceutical plants: comparative data from the Island of Bali (Indonesia). J Cult Herit. 2016;18:342–8. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.culher.2015.06.006.

  26. García-Flores J, González-Espinosa M, Lindig-Cisneros R, Casas A. Traditional medicinal knowledge of tropical trees and its value for restoration of tropical forests. Bot Sci. 2019;97(3):336–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Nieman WA. Culture, conflict and cuisine: a quantitative assessment of terrestrial vertebrate off-take at the human-wildlife interface. Stellenbosch University; 2018.

  28. Allendorf TD, Swe KK, Aung M, Thorsen A. Community use and perceptions of a biodiversity corridor in Myanmar’s threatened southern forests. Glob Ecol Conserv. 2018;15:e00409. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.gecco.2018.e00409.

  29. Pelegrina-López A, Ocaña-Peinado FM, Henares-Civantos I, Rosúa-Campos JL, Serrano-Bernardo FA. Analyzing social perception as a key factor in the management of protected areas: the case of the Sierra Nevada Protected Area (S Spain). J Environ Plan Manag. 2018;61(1):124–42. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1080/09640568.2017.1291413.

  30. Moutouama FT, Biaou SSH, Kyereh B, Asante WA, Natta AK. Factors shaping local people’s perception of ecosystem services in the Atacora Chain of Mountains, a biodiversity hotspot in northern Benin. J Ethnobiol Ethnomed. 2019;15(1):1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  31. Dirzo R, Miranda A. El límite boreal de la selva tropical húmeda en el continente americano: contracción de la vegetación y solución de una controversia. Interciencia. 1991;16(5):240–7.

    Google Scholar 

  32. Estrada A, Coates-Estrada R. Some observations on the present distribution and conservation of Alouatta and Ateles in Southern Mexico. Am J Primatol. 1984;7(2):133–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Cristóbal-Azkarate J, Dunn JC. Lessons from Los Tuxtlas: 30 Years of research into primates in fragments. In: Marsh LK, Chapman CA, editors. Primates in fragments: complexity and resilience. New York: Springer; 2013. p. 75–88.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  34. Rodríguez-Luna E, Gómez-Pompa A, López J, Velázquez RN, Aguilar Y, Vázquez M. Atlas de los espacios naturales protegidos de Veracruz, México. Ciudad de México: Universidad Veracruzana; 2011. p. 250.

    Google Scholar 

  35. INPI. Popolucas de la Sierra (Veracruz) [Internet]. Atlas de los pueblos indígenas de México. 2020 [cited 2021 Mar 20]. Available from: http://atlas.inpi.gob.mx/?page_id=8233

  36. CONANP. Programa de Conservación y Manejo Reserva de la Biosfera Los Tuxtlas. México, D.F.: CONANP & SEMARNAT; 2006. 293 p.

  37. Paré L, Velázquez E, Gutiérrez R. La ganadería en la Sierra de Santa Marta: una primera aproximación. In: Barrera N, Rodríguez H, editors. Desarrollo y medio ambiente en Veracruz Impactos económicos, ecológicos y culturales de la ganadería en Veracruz. México: Fundación Friedrich Ebert; 1993. p. 129–65.

    Google Scholar 

  38. Guevara S, Laborde J, Sánchez-Ríos G. Los Tuxtlas, El Paisaje de la Sierra. Xalapa: Instituto de Ecología AC y Unión Europea; 2004. 228 p.

  39. Vega-Vela V, Muñóz-Robles CA, Rodríguez-Luna E, López-Acosta JC, Serna LR. Análisis de la fragmentación del paisaje de la Reserva de la Biosfera Los Tuxtlas, Veracruz. México Ecosistemas y Recur Agropecu. 2018;5(14):227–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Van Cuong C, Dart P, Hockings M. Biosphere reserves: Attributes for success. J Environ Manage. 2017;188:9–17. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.jenvman.2016.11.069.

  41. Velázquez E, Ramírez F. Disputas y Adaptaciones en Torno al Uso de los Recursos en la Reserva de la Biosfera “Los Tuxtlas”, Veracruz (México). Rev Estud e Pesqui sobre as Américas. 2015;9(3):1–28.

    Google Scholar 

  42. Chowdhury SMH, Gudmundsson C, Izumiyama S, Koike M, Nazia N, Rana MP, et al. Community attitudes toward forest conservation programs through collaborative protected area management in Bangladesh. Environ Dev Sustain. 2014;16(6):1235–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Costa-Neto EM, Santos-Fita D, Vargas-Clavijo M. Manual de Etnozoología: una guía teórico-práctica para investigar la interconexión del ser humano con los animales. Tundra Ed. Valencia, España; 2009. 285 p.

  44. González-Zamora A. Patrones de utilización de fragmentos de selva por el mono arana (Ateles geoffroyi vellerosus) en el sur de Veracruz. Instituto de Ecología AC, Xalapa; 2003.

  45. Soto M. El Clima. In: Laborde J, Sánchez G, editors. Los Tuxtlas: El paisaje de la sierra. Instituto de Ecología AC and European Union; 2004. p. 195–200.

  46. Castillo-Campos G, Laborde J. No Title. In: Laborde J, Sánchez G, editors. Los Tuxtlas: El paisaje de la sierra. Instituto de Ecología AC and European Union; 2004. p. 231–263.

  47. IMSS. Censo oficial: Delegación regional Veracruz Sur. Xalapa; 2017.

  48. Cano EJ, Medinaceli A, Sanabria OL, Argueta A. Código de Ética para la Investigación, la Investigación-Acción y la Colaboración Etnocientífica en América Latina. Versión Dos Etnobiología. 2015;13(4):5–6.

    Google Scholar 

  49. Pinto-Marroquin M, Serio-Silva JC. Perception and Uses of Primates Among Popoluca Indigenous People in Los Tuxtlas, Mexico. In: Urbani B, Lizarralde M, editors. Neotropical Ethnoprimatology: Indigenous Peoples’ Perceptions of and Interactions with Nonhuman Primates. Cham, Switzerland: Springer; 2020. p. 3–20. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1007/978-3-030-27504-4_1.

  50. Alonso-Aguilar LE, Montoya A, Kong A, Estrada-Torres A, Garibay-Orijel R. The cultural significance of wild mushrooms in San Mateo Huexoyucan, Tlaxcala. Mexico J Ethnobiol Ethnomed. 2014;10(1):1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Pieroni A. Evaluation of the cultural significance of wild food botanicals traditionally consumed in Northwestern Tuscany, Italy. J Ethnobiol. 2001;21(1):89–104.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Friedman J, Yaniv Z, Dafni A, Palewitch D. A preliminary classification of the healing potential of medicinal plants, based on a rational analysis of an ethnopharmacological field survey among Bedouins in the Negev Desert. Israel J Ethnopharmacol. 1986;16(2–3):275–87.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Burnham KP, Anderson DR. Model selection and multimodel inference: a practical information-theoretic approach, vol. 2. New York: Springer; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  54. Pinheiro J, Bates D, DebRoy S, Sarkar D, Team RC. nlme: Linear and Nonlinear Mixed Effects Models. R package version 3.1–128 [Internet]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2016. http://cran.rproject.org/package¼nlme.

  55. Bolker B. bbmle: Tools for general maximum likelihood estimation. R package version 1.0.23.1. 2020.

  56. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Viena, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013.

  57. Solórzano B, Rodríguez E. Cambios Demograficos en Poblaciones de Primates de la Region Sur de Los Tuxtlas, Mexico: Analisis Longitudinal 1985–2008. Neotrop Primates. 2010;17(1):1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Arroyo-Rodríguez V, Dias PAD. Effects of habitat fragmentation and disturbance on howler monkeys: a review. Am J Primatol. 2010;72(1):1–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Estrada A, Coates-Estrada R. Tropical rain forest fragmentation and wild populations of p.pdf. Int J Primatol. 1996;17(5):759–83.

  60. Estrada A. La Estación Los Tuxtlas como motor generador de investigaciones con primates silvestres en el sur de México. In: Reynoso V., Coates R, editors. Avances y Perspectivas de la Investigacion de Bosques Tropicales y sus Alrededores: Los Tuxtlas [Internet]. Ciudad de México: Universidad Autónoma de México; 2011. p. 1–24. Available from: http://www.primatesmx.com/semblanzaprimatesltaestrada09.pdf.

  61. Cristóbal-Azkarate J, Domingo-Balcells C, Dunn JC, Vea-Baro J. Evolución demográfica de monos aulladores en fragmentos de selva en Los Tuxtlas. In: Dias PAD, Negrín AR, Espinosa DC, editors. La conservación de los primates en México. Xalapa: Consejo Veracruzano de Ciencia y Tecnología; 2011. p. 27–43.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Serio-Silva JC. Las Islas de los Changos (the Monkey Islands): The economic impact of ecotourism in the region of Los Tuxtlas, Veracruz, Mexico. Am J Primatol. 2006;68(5):499–506.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Matheson MD. Primate Tourism. Int Encycl Primatol. 2017;1–8.

  64. Klailova M, Hodgkinson C, Lee PC. Behavioral responses of one western lowland gorilla (Gorilla gorilla gorilla) group at Bai Hokou, Central African Republic, to tourists, researchers and trackers. Am J Primatol. 2010;72(10):897–906.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Fuentes A, Southern M, Suaryana KG. Monkey forests and human landscapes: Is extensive sympatry sustainable for Homo sapiens and Macaca fascicularis in Bali? In: Paterson JD, Wallis J, editors. Commensalism and conflict: the primate-human interface. American Society of Primatology: Norman; 2005. p. 168–95.

    Google Scholar 

  66. Fishman J. What do you lose when you lose your language? In: Cantoni G, editor. Stabilizing indigenous languages. Flagstaff: Center for Excellence in Education, Northern Arizona University; 1996. p. 81–90.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Basso E. The Kalapalo Indians of Central Brazil. Rhinehart and Winston, New York: Holt; 1973.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Kracke WH. Force and Persuasion. Leadership in an Amazonian Society. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press; 1978.

  69. Papworth S, Milner-Gulland EJ, Slocombe K. The natural place to begin: the ethnoprimatology of the Waorani. Am J Primatol. 2013;75(11):1117–28.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Parathian HE, Maldonado AM. Human-nonhuman primate interactions amongst Tikuna People: perceptions and local initiatives for resource management in Amacayacu in the Colombian Amazon. Am J Primatol. 2010;72(10):855–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  71. Alves RRN, Souto WMS, Barboza RRD. Primates in traditional folk medicine: a world overview. Mamm Rev. 2010;40(2):155–80.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Camargo A. The Path of Beliefs. Some Determinants of Religious Mobility in Latin America. Front Norte. 2019;31(3):1–14.

  73. de León L. Indigenous language policy and education in Mexico. Lang policy Polit issues Educ. 2017;415–33.

  74. Waldinger M. The Long-Run Effects of Missionary Orders in Mexico. J Dev Econ [Internet]. 2016;127:355–78. https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1016/j.jdeveco.2016.12.010.

  75. Medrano C, Suárez V. When monkeys were humans: narratives of the relationship between primates and the Qom (Toba) people of the Gran Chaco of Argentina. In: Urbani B, Lizarralde M, editors. Neotropical ethnoprimatology. Cham, Switzerland: Springer; 2020. p. 363–76.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  76. El VE. territorio de los popolucas de Soteapan, Veracruz: transformaciones en la organización y apropiación del espacio. Relac Estud Hist y Soc. 2001;22(87):16–47.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Moorman RS. Benefits of local residents visiting La Selva biological station, Costa Rica. Environ Conserv. 2006;89–99.

  78. Leisher C, Temsah G, Booker F, Day M, Samberg L, Prosnitz D, et al. Does the gender composition of forest and fishery management groups affect resource governance and conservation outcomes? A systematics map. Environ Evid. 2016;5(1):1–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Franquesa-Soler M, Jorge-Sales L, Aristizabal JF, Moreno-Casasola P, Serio-Silva JC. Evidence-based conservation education in Mexican communities: Connecting arts and science. PLoS ONE. 2020;15(2):1–18.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  80. MacKenzie CA, Salerno J, Hartter J, Chapman CA, Reyna R, Tumusiime DM, et al. Changing perceptions of protected area benefits and problems around Kibale National Park, Uganda. J Environ Manag. 2017;200:217–28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  81. De la Torre S, Yepez P, Payaguaje A. No titlelinguistic, cultural, and environmental aspects of ethnoprimatological knowledge among the Lokono, Kari’na, and Warao of the Moruca River (Guyana). In: Urbani B, Lizarralde M, editors. Neotropical ethnoprimatology. Springer: Cham; 2020. p. 239–62.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Chapman CA, DeLuycker A, Reyna-Hurtado RA, Serio-Silva JC, Smith TB, Strier KB, et al. Safeguarding biodiversity: What is perceived as working, according to the conservation community? Oryx. 2016;50(2):302–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  83. Maréchal L, Maclarnon A, Majolo B, Semple S. Primates’ behavioural responses to tourists: evidence for a trade-off between potential risks and benefits. Sci Reports Nat Publ Gr. 2016;6(32465). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1038/srep32465.

  84. Karst H. “This is a holy place of Ama Jomo”: buen vivir, indigenous voices and ecotourism development in a protected area of Bhutan. J Sustain Tour. 2017;25(6):746–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  85. Krüger O. The role of ecotourism in conservation: Panacea or Pandora’s box? Biodivers Conserv. 2005;14(3):579–600.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  86. Jost Robinson CA, Remis MJ. Engaging holism: exploring multispecies approaches in ethnoprimatology. Int J Primatol. 2018;39(5):776–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Kohn E. How Forests Think: Toward an Anthropology Beyond the Human [Internet]. University of California Press; 2013. 288 p. https://0-www-ucpress-edu.brum.beds.ac.uk/book/9780520276116/how-forests-think.

  88. Descola P. “All too human (still)” A comment on Eduardo Kohn’s How forests think. HAU J Ethnogr Theory. 2014;4(2):267–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Zent S, Zent E. Co-ecology of Jotï, primates, and other people: a multispecies ethnography in the Venezuelan Guayana. In: Urbani B, Lizarralde M, editors. Neotropical Ethnoprimatology. Cham: Springer; 2020. p. 161–97.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  90. Albuquerque UP, Ludwig D, Feitosa IS, de Moura JMB, de Medeiros PM, Gonçalves PHS, et al. Addressing social-ecological systems across temporal and spatial scales: a conceptual synthesis for ethnobiology. Hum Ecol. 2020;48(5):557–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  91. Fuentes A, Wolfe LD. Primates face to face: the conservation implications of human-nonhuman primate interconnections [Internet]. Vol. 29. Cambridge University Press; 2002. 340 p. www.cambridge.org/9780521791090.

  92. Cormier L. Kinship with monkeys [Internet]. New York: Columbia University Press; 2003. 234 p. http://cup.columbia.edu/book/kinship-with-monkeys/9780231125253.

  93. Rosales-Meda M, Hermes MS. Representation and signification of primates in Maya-Q’eqchi’Cosmovision and Implications for their conservation in Northwestern Guatemala. In: Urbani B, Lizarralde M, editors. Neotropical ethnoprimatology. Cham: Springer; 2020. p. 69–85.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  94. Urquiza-Haas EG, Martínez RIO, Kotrschal K. Mental state attribution to nonhuman primates and other animals by rural inhabitants of the community of Conhuas near the Calakmul biosphere reserve in the Yucatan peninsula, Mexico. In: Urbani B, Lizarralde M, editors. Neotropical ethnoprimatology. Springer: Cham; 2020. p. 21–48.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We are thankful to the all the participants and authorities in the communities of Piedra Labrada and Los Mangos. We thank Concepción Acosta, Lorenzo Arteaga, Flavio Albino, Jeison Rosero, and Beto and Berta Palma for their valuable support in the field. We thank to Rolando González Trapaga for the technical support during data process, to Mary E. Glenn (Humboldt State University) and Alejandra Duarte for their review, comments and suggestions of the final version of the manuscript. To Julien Rouyrre, Stephanie Pouls (Akumal Monkey Sanctuary & Rescued Animals) and Sarah Otterstrom (Paso Pacífico) for the language editing. Finally, we would like to thank the INECOL for their support on the development of this project, as well as CONACYT for the scholarship (number 671519) they granted the first author.

Funding

This study was funded by Consejo Nacional de Ciencia y Tecnología, México, Grant Number: 671519.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MPM designed the study, analyzed and interpreted the data, and wrote the original draft. JFA performed the statistics tests, helped to analyze data and edited the manuscripts. YGV participated in the study design and provided guidance for data collection. FRS participated in the study design, provided guidance for data collection and edited the manuscripts. JCSS participated in the study design, funding field work and getting/funding field assistants, and edited the manuscripts. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to John F. Aristizabal or Juan Carlos Serio-Silva.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

No ethics approval was needed for this study. The participants gave their oral consent to take part in the survey.

Consent for publication

The participants were informed that their responses will be published in scientific documents and agreed with that.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary Information

Additional file 1

. Cultural significance of primates’ questionnaire. Applied to the participants of the community inside (Piedra Labrada, N = 81) and the community outside (Los Mangos, N = 91) of Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve (Additional file 1).

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Pinto-Marroquin, M., Aristizabal, J.F., García-Del Valle, Y. et al. The Primate Cultural Significance Index: applications with Popoluca Indigenous people at Los Tuxtlas Biosphere Reserve. J Ethnobiology Ethnomedicine 17, 57 (2021). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13002-021-00483-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s13002-021-00483-8

Keywords